L. Griffin @lngriffin25
tweets
Her article is out and published
''Audience Reactions to Climate Change and Science in Disaster Cli-fi Films: A Qualitative Analysis''
Audience Reactions to Climate Change and Science in Disaster Cli-fi Films: A Qualitative Analysis
Abstract
Little scholarly attention has been paid to how audiences interpret pop culture messages about climate. This paper addresses this issue by taking up the case of disaster cli-fi films and exploring how audiences react to film representations of climate change. It draws on data from focus groups to evaluate audience responses to disaster cli-fi films. Analysis reveals that by only briefly discussing climate change in their plotlines, the films weaken their environmental message. The paper concludes with a discussion of the effects of disaster cli-fi films on environmental attitudes and suggestions for further research.
Keywords
climate change; narratives; mass media; public understanding of science; focus groups
Full Text:
PDFReferences
Barriga, C. A., Shapiro, M. A., & Fernandez, M. L. (2010). Science information in fictional movies: Effects of context and gender. Science Communication, 32(1), 3–24. doi:10.1177/1075547009340338
Beattie, G., Sale, L., & McGuire, L. (2011). An inconvenient truth? Can a film really affect psychological mood and our explicit attitudes towards climate change? Semiotica, 187(1–4), 105–125. doi:10.1515/semi.2011.066
Bloom, D. (2017). The Cli-Fi Report. Retrieved from http://cli-fi.net/
Boykoff, J. M., & Boykoff, M. T. (2007). Climate change and journalistic norms: A case-study of US mass-media coverage. Geoforum, 38(6), 1190–1204. doi:10.1016/j.geoforum.2007.01.008
Boykoff, M. T., & Rajan, S. R. (2007). Signals and noise. Mass-media coverage of climate change in the USA and the UK. EMBO Reports, 8(3), 207–211. doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400924
Braddock, K., & Dillard, J. P. (2016). Meta-analytic evidence for the persuasive effect of narratives on beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Communication Monographs, 83(4), 446–467. doi:10.1080/03637751.2015.1128555
Braddock, K., & Horgan, J. (2016). Towards a guide for constructing and disseminating counternarratives to reduce support for terrorism. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 39(5), 381–404. doi:10.1080/1057610X.2015.1116277
Brulle, R. J., Carmichael, J., & Jenkins, J. C. (2012). Shifting public opinion on climate change: An empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the U.S., 2002-2010. Climatic Change, 114(1), 169. doi:10.1007/s10584-012-0403-y
Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories: A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18(2), 255–280. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00322.x
Busselle, R. W., & Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychology, 12(4), 321–347. doi:10.1080/15213260903287259
Butler, A. C., Zaromb, F. M., Lyle, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Using popular films to enhance classroom learning: The good, the bad, and the interesting. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1161–1168. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02410.x
Caputo, N. M., & Rouner, D. (2011). Narrative processing of entertainment media and mental illness stigma. Health Communication, 26(7), 595–604. doi:10.1080/10410236.2011.560787
Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Charmaz, K. (2008). Grounded theory as an emergent method. In S. N. Hesse-Biber & P. Leavy (Eds.), Handbook of emergent methods. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Corbett, J., & Durfee, J. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science: Media representations of global warming. Science Communication, 26(2), 129–151. doi:10.1177/1075547004270234
Dahlstrom, M. F. (2010). The role of causality in information acceptance in narratives: An example from science communication. Communication Research, 37(6), 857–875. doi:10.1177/0093650210362683
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 47(1), 1–15. doi:10.1086/268763
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701–721. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.701
Hall, A. (2003). Reading realism: Audiences’ evaluations of the reality of media texts. Journal of Communication, 53(4), 624–641. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb02914.x
Hart, P. S., & Leiserowitz, A. (2009). Finding the teachable moment: An analysis of information-seeking behavior on global warming related websites during the release of The Day After Tomorrow. Environmental Communication - A Journal of Nature and Culture, 3(3), 355–366. doi:10.1080/17524030903265823
Howell, R. A. (2011). Lights, camera...action? Altered attitudes and behaviour in response to the climate change film The Age of Stupid. Global Environmental Change, 21(1), 177–187.
doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.09.004
Kahan, D. (2012). Why we are poles apart on climate change: The problem isn’t the public’s reasoning capacity; it’s the polluted science-communication environment that drives people apart. Nature, 488(7411), 255. doi:10.1038/488255a
Kahan, D., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174. doi:10.1080/13669877.2010.511246
Kaplan, E. A. (2015). Climate trauma: Foreseeing the future in dystopian film and fiction. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Kirby, D. A. (2003a). Science consultants, fictional films, and scientific practice. Social Studies of Science, 33(2), 231–268. doi:10.1177/03063127030332015
Kirby, D. A. (2003b). Scientists on the set: Science consultants and the communication of science in visual fiction. Public Understanding of Science, 12(3), 261–278.
doi:10.1177/0963662503123005
Konijn, E. A., van der Molen, J. H. W., & van Nes, S. (2009). Emotions bias perceptions of realism in audiovisual media: Why we may take fiction for real. Discourse Processes, 46(4), 309–340. doi:10.1080/01638530902728546
Krueger, R. A., & Casey, M. A. (2009). Focus groups: A practical guide for applied research. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications.
Leiserowitz, A. (2004). Before and after the day after tomorrow: A US study of climate change risk perception. Environment, 46(9), 22–37.
Lowe, T., Brown, K., Dessai, S., de Franca Doria, M., Haynes, K., & Vincent, K. (2006). Does tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public perceptions of climate change. Public Understanding of Science, 15(4), 435–457. doi:10.1177/0963662506063796
Marsh, E. J., Butler, A. C., & Umanath, S. (2012). Using fictional sources in the classroom: Applications from cognitive psychology. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 449–469. doi:10.1007/s10648-012-9204-0
Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger III, H. L. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 519–536. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00092-5
Merchant, B. (2013). Behold the Rise of Dystopian “Cli-Fi.” Retrieved from https://motherboard.vice.com/en_us/article/ypp7nj/behold-the-rise-of-cli-fi
Morgan, D. L. (1993). Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Morgan, D. L. (1996). Focus groups. Annual Review of Sociology, 22(1), 129–152. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.22.1.129
Moyer-Guse, E. (2008). Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: Explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment-education messages. Communication Theory, 18(3), 407–425. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00328.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2011). An experimental test of the effects of fictional framing on attitudes. Social Science Quarterly, 92(1), 79–99. doi:10.1111/j.1540-6237.2011.00758.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2013). The implications of fictional media for political beliefs. American Politics Research, 41(1), 122–146. doi:10.1177/1532673X12453758
Murphy, P. (2014). Directing the weather, producing the climate. In Alex, R.S., Deborah, S.S., & Sachindev, P.S. (Eds.), Culture and media: Ecocritical explorations. Newcastle upon Tyne, UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Murray, R. L., & Heumann, J. K. (2009). Ecology and popular film: Cinema on the edge. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Ohlheiser, A. (2017, September 11). Analysis | A running list of viral hoaxes about Irma — including one shared by the White House. The Washington Post Retrieved from https://www.washingtonpost.com
Parker, T. (2006). ManBearPig. Comedy Central. Retrieved from http://southpark.cc.com/full-episodes/s10e06-manbearpig
Paul, B., Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (2000). The third-person effect: A meta-analysis of the perceptual hypothesis. Mass Communication and Society, 3(1), 57–85. doi:10.1207/S15327825MCS0301_04
Porter, T. (2017, September 9). Here’s some of the fake news and false rumors about Hurricane Irma. Retrieved from http://www.newsweek.com/hurricane-irma-fake-news-false-rumors-spread-storm-662419
Rooney, B., Benson, C., & Hennessy, E. (2012). The apparent reality of movies and emotional arousal: A study using physiological and self-report measures. Poetics, 40, 405–422. doi:10.1016/j.poetic.2012.07.004
Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (1999). The third-person effect. Communication Research, 26(5), 523. doi:10.1177/009365099026005001
Shen, F., Ahern, L., & Baker, M. (2014). Stories that count: Influence of news narratives on issue attitudes. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 91(1), 98–117. doi:10.1177/1077699013514414
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). The rise of global warming skepticism: Exploring affective image associations in the United States over time. Risk Analysis, 32(6), 1021–1032. doi:10.1605/01.301-0019863810.2012
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2014). The role of emotion in global warming policy support and opposition. Risk Analysis, 34(5), 937–948. doi:10.1111/risa.12140
Svoboda, M. (2014). The Long Melt: The Lingering Influence of The Day After Tomorrow. Retrieved from http://www.yaleclimateconnections.org/2014/11/the-long-melt-the-lingering-influence-of-the-day-after-tomorrow/
Svoboda, M. (2016). Cli-fi on the screen(s): Patterns in the representations of climate change in fictional films. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 7(1), 43–64.
doi:10.1002/wcc.381
Tan, E. S. H. (2008). Entertainment is emotion: The functional architecture of the entertainment experience. Media Psychology, 11(1), 28–51. doi:10.1080/15213260701853161
Beattie, G., Sale, L., & McGuire, L. (2011). An inconvenient truth? Can a film really affect psychological mood and our explicit attitudes towards climate change? Semiotica, 187(1–4), 105–125. doi:10.1515/semi.2011.066
Bloom, D. (2017). The Cli-Fi Report. Retrieved from http://cli-fi.net/
Boykoff, J. M., & Boykoff, M. T. (2007). Climate change and journalistic norms: A case-study of US mass-media coverage. Geoforum, 38(6), 1190–1204. doi:10.1016/j.geoforum.2007.01.008
Boykoff, M. T., & Rajan, S. R. (2007). Signals and noise. Mass-media coverage of climate change in the USA and the UK. EMBO Reports, 8(3), 207–211. doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400924
Braddock, K., & Dillard, J. P. (2016). Meta-analytic evidence for the persuasive effect of narratives on beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Communication Monographs, 83(4), 446–467. doi:10.1080/03637751.2015.1128555
Braddock, K., & Horgan, J. (2016). Towards a guide for constructing and disseminating counternarratives to reduce support for terrorism. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 39(5), 381–404. doi:10.1080/1057610X.2015.1116277
Brulle, R. J., Carmichael, J., & Jenkins, J. C. (2012). Shifting public opinion on climate change: An empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the U.S., 2002-2010. Climatic Change, 114(1), 169. doi:10.1007/s10584-012-0403-y
Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories: A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18(2), 255–280. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00322.x
Busselle, R. W., & Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychology, 12(4), 321–347. doi:10.1080/15213260903287259
Butler, A. C., Zaromb, F. M., Lyle, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Using popular films to enhance classroom learning: The good, the bad, and the interesting. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1161–1168. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02410.x
Caputo, N. M., & Rouner, D. (2011). Narrative processing of entertainment media and mental illness stigma. Health Communication, 26(7), 595–604. doi:10.1080/10410236.2011.560787
Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Charmaz, K. (2008). Grounded theory as an emergent method. In S. N. Hesse-Biber & P. Leavy (Eds.), Handbook of emergent methods. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Corbett, J., & Durfee, J. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science: Media representations of global warming. Science Communication, 26(2), 129–151. doi:10.1177/1075547004270234
Dahlstrom, M. F. (2010). The role of causality in information acceptance in narratives: An example from science communication. Communication Research, 37(6), 857–875. doi:10.1177/0093650210362683
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 47(1), 1–15. doi:10.1086/268763
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701–721. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.701
Hall, A. (2003). Reading realism: Audiences’ evaluations of the reality of media texts. Journal of Communication, 53(4), 624–641. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb02914.x
Hart, P. S., & Leiserowitz, A. (2009). Finding the teachable moment: An analysis of information-seeking behavior on global warming related websites during the release of The Day After Tomorrow. Environmental Communication - A Journal of Nature and Culture, 3(3), 355–366. doi:10.1080/17524030903265823
Howell, R. A. (2011). Lights, camera...action? Altered attitudes and behaviour in response to the climate change film The Age of Stupid. Global Environmental Change, 21(1), 177–187.
doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.09.004
Kahan, D. (2012). Why we are poles apart on climate change: The problem isn’t the public’s reasoning capacity; it’s the polluted science-communication environment that drives people apart. Nature, 488(7411), 255. doi:10.1038/488255a
Kahan, D., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174. doi:10.1080/13669877.2010.511246
Kaplan, E. A. (2015). Climate trauma: Foreseeing the future in dystopian film and fiction. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Kirby, D. A. (2003a). Science consultants, fictional films, and scientific practice. Social Studies of Science, 33(2), 231–268. doi:10.1177/03063127030332015
Kirby, D. A. (2003b). Scientists on the set: Science consultants and the communication of science in visual fiction. Public Understanding of Science, 12(3), 261–278.
doi:10.1177/0963662503123005
Konijn, E. A., van der Molen, J. H. W., & van Nes, S. (2009). Emotions bias perceptions of realism in audiovisual media: Why we may take fiction for real. Discourse Processes, 46(4), 309–340. doi:10.1080/01638530902728546
Krueger, R. A., & Casey, M. A. (2009). Focus groups: A practical guide for applied research. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications.
Leiserowitz, A. (2004). Before and after the day after tomorrow: A US study of climate change risk perception. Environment, 46(9), 22–37.
Lowe, T., Brown, K., Dessai, S., de Franca Doria, M., Haynes, K., & Vincent, K. (2006). Does tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public perceptions of climate change. Public Understanding of Science, 15(4), 435–457. doi:10.1177/0963662506063796
Marsh, E. J., Butler, A. C., & Umanath, S. (2012). Using fictional sources in the classroom: Applications from cognitive psychology. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 449–469. doi:10.1007/s10648-012-9204-0
Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger III, H. L. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 519–536. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00092-5
Merchant, B. (2013). Behold the Rise of Dystopian “Cli-Fi.” Retrieved from https://motherboard.vice.com/en_us/article/ypp7nj/behold-the-rise-of-cli-fi
Morgan, D. L. (1993). Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Morgan, D. L. (1996). Focus groups. Annual Review of Sociology, 22(1), 129–152. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.22.1.129
Moyer-Guse, E. (2008). Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: Explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment-education messages. Communication Theory, 18(3), 407–425. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00328.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2011). An experimental test of the effects of fictional framing on attitudes. Social Science Quarterly, 92(1), 79–99. doi:10.1111/j.1540-6237.2011.00758.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2013). The implications of fictional media for political beliefs. American Politics Research, 41(1), 122–146. doi:10.1177/1532673X12453758
Murphy, P. (2014). Directing the weather, producing the climate. In Alex, R.S., Deborah, S.S., & Sachindev, P.S. (Eds.), Culture and media: Ecocritical explorations. Newcastle upon Tyne, UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Murray, R. L., & Heumann, J. K. (2009). Ecology and popular film: Cinema on the edge. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Ohlheiser, A. (2017, September 11). Analysis | A running list of viral hoaxes about Irma — including one shared by the White House. The Washington Post Retrieved from https://www.washingtonpost.com
Parker, T. (2006). ManBearPig. Comedy Central. Retrieved from http://southpark.cc.com/full-episodes/s10e06-manbearpig
Paul, B., Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (2000). The third-person effect: A meta-analysis of the perceptual hypothesis. Mass Communication and Society, 3(1), 57–85. doi:10.1207/S15327825MCS0301_04
Porter, T. (2017, September 9). Here’s some of the fake news and false rumors about Hurricane Irma. Retrieved from http://www.newsweek.com/hurricane-irma-fake-news-false-rumors-spread-storm-662419
Rooney, B., Benson, C., & Hennessy, E. (2012). The apparent reality of movies and emotional arousal: A study using physiological and self-report measures. Poetics, 40, 405–422. doi:10.1016/j.poetic.2012.07.004
Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (1999). The third-person effect. Communication Research, 26(5), 523. doi:10.1177/009365099026005001
Shen, F., Ahern, L., & Baker, M. (2014). Stories that count: Influence of news narratives on issue attitudes. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 91(1), 98–117. doi:10.1177/1077699013514414
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). The rise of global warming skepticism: Exploring affective image associations in the United States over time. Risk Analysis, 32(6), 1021–1032. doi:10.1605/01.301-0019863810.2012
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2014). The role of emotion in global warming policy support and opposition. Risk Analysis, 34(5), 937–948. doi:10.1111/risa.12140
Svoboda, M. (2014). The Long Melt: The Lingering Influence of The Day After Tomorrow. Retrieved from http://www.yaleclimateconnections.org/2014/11/the-long-melt-the-lingering-influence-of-the-day-after-tomorrow/
Svoboda, M. (2016). Cli-fi on the screen(s): Patterns in the representations of climate change in fictional films. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 7(1), 43–64.
doi:10.1002/wcc.381
Tan, E. S. H. (2008). Entertainment is emotion: The functional architecture of the entertainment experience. Media Psychology, 11(1), 28–51. doi:10.1080/15213260701853161
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.
Journal of Public Interest Communications, Vol. 1 Issue 2, 2017
Introduction
The mass media are a crucial source of information regarding the causes and predicted effects of
anthropogenic climate change for many people (J. M. Boykoff & Boykoff, 2007; M. T. Boykoff
& Rajan, 2007; Brulle, Carmichael, & Jenkins, 2012; Corbett & Durfee, 2004). As a result,
researchers have begun to analyze the discourse surrounding climate change in the media,
particularly the news media. Although these studies have produced valuable information, they do
not address the effects of non-news media—namely, fictional narratives—on climate change
attitudes and beliefs.
Audience Reactions to Climate Change and Science in
Disaster Cli-fi Films: A Qualitative Analysis
Lauren N. Griffin
University of Florida
Article Information
Received: July 17, 2017
Accepted: October 8, 2017
Published online: December 20, 2017
Abstract
Little scholarly attention has been paid to how audiences
interpret pop culture messages about climate. This paper
addresses this issue by taking up the case of disaster cli-fi
films and exploring how audiences react to film
representations of climate change. It draws on data from
focus groups to evaluate audience responses to disaster
cli-fi films. Analysis reveals that by only briefly
discussing climate change in their plotlines, the films
weaken their environmental message. The paper
concludes with a discussion of the effects of disaster cli-fi
films on environmental attitudes and suggestions for
further research.
Journal of Public Interest Communications
ISSN (online): 2573-4342
Journal homepage: http://journals.fcla.edu/jpic/
Keywords
Climate change
Narratives
Mass media
Public understanding of science
Focus Groups
*Please send correspondences about this article to Lauren N. Griffin, College of Journalism and
Communications, University of Florida. E-mail: lngriffin@ufl.edu. Copyright Griffin 2017. This work is
licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-Non Commercial 3.0 United States License. To view a copy of
this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/us/orsend a letter to Creative Commons, PO
Box 1866, Mountain View, CA 94042, USA.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
134
Climate fiction, or “cli-fi,” (Bloom, 2017) is an emerging area of exploration for climate
change communications. Cli-fi is a growing presence in literature, cinema, and gaming and
consists of narratives that examine the causes, effects, and implications of anthropogenic climate
change (Merchant, 2013). Braddock and Horgan describe narratives as “any cohesive and
coherent account of events with an identifiable beginning, middle, and end about characters
engaged in actions that result in questions or conflicts for which answers or resolutions are
provided” (2016, pp. 382–383).
Researchers have begun to address cli-fi’s impact on public attitudes and beliefs about
climate change (Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe, Brown, Dessai, de Franca, Doria, Haynes, & Vincent,
2006; Svoboda, 2014, 2016). However, so far these approaches have mainly used quantitative
measures to examine audience responses to cli-fi. A numerical approach leaves the possibility
open that significant aspects of the interpretive process are missing from the current literature.
This paper addresses this gap by undertaking a qualitative examination of how audiences
interpret climate fiction using the subject of disaster cli-fi films.
I provide background by briefly describing cli-fi and how it relates to disaster films. I then
discuss narrative persuasion and how narratives have been used to communicate facts. I then
provide an overview of the challenge of communicating science to the public, followed by a
review of the connection between realism and emotion in driving narrative transportation. I then
review the methods and data for the study and discuss the significant impacts of film on audience
emotions, weighing this against the question of realism in film. I suggest that this question of
balance is important for understanding audience responses to climate disaster films as they
inform audience views on climate change.
I use data from focus groups of disaster cli-fi film audiences to qualitatively evaluate their
responses to climate change and climate science in three films: NYC Tornado Terror (2008),
Lightning: Bolts of Destruction (2003), and F6 Twister (also released under the title Christmas
Twister) (2012). Discussions centered on audiences’ emotional responses to the films and their
evaluation of the films’ factual accuracy and were analyzed using grounded theory. My analysis
suggests that films weaken any environmental message present by only discussing climate
change in a cursory way. My analysis also revealed audience skepticism about the speed and
severity of climate disasters depicted in the films and the problem of the lone scientist. I
conclude with a discussion of the potential effects of disaster cli-fi films on environmental
attitudes and suggestions for further research.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
135
Background
What is cli-fi?
Cli-fi is a portmanteau of the words climate and fiction coined by Dan Bloom (Bloom, 2017).
Climate change and other environmental problems are increasingly featured in fictional films
(Murray & Heumann, 2009) such as the blockbusters The Day After Tomorrow (2006) and
Snowpiercer (2013) and lower-budget productions such as Category 6 (2004). The cli-fi film
genre is broad and contains films that touch on climate change only briefly (Svoboda, 2016).
Most research on cli-fi films examines better-known productions such as The Day After
Tomorrow (Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al., 2006; Svoboda, 2014) or The Age of Stupid (Howell,
2011). Kaplan explores the broader genre and concludes cli-fi is evidence of environmental pretrauma,
or anxiety about things that will happen in the future (2015). Svoboda examines the
history of cli-fi films and begins the process of classifying these films by the type of disaster they
feature (Svoboda, 2016).
The intersection between disaster films and cli-fi
I focus here on disaster cli-fi films, a type of cli-fi that intersects with the disaster film genre. By
definition, cli-fi narratives discuss climate change, but they do not always depict natural
disasters. Mad Max: Fury Road (2015) implies climate change contributed to the collapse of
ecosystems but does not show this happening in the film itself. Disaster films focus on disasters
as they happen but do not always mention climate change. Twister (1996) is driven by storm
chasers following destructive tornadoes but does not feature climate change. Disaster cli-fi films,
on the other hand, show climate change-driven disasters as they unfold. These films frequently
depict climate change inaccurately—compressing long-term environmental changes into a few
hours, for instance (Murphy, 2014)—but they relate natural disasters to climate change to a
greater or lesser degree.
The persuasiveness of narratives
Communication scholarship suggests narratives can influence attitudes and beliefs of audiences
(Braddock & Dillard, 2016; Mulligan & Habel, 2011, 2013). Facts—including science facts—
presented in narratives may appear more real to audiences than facts not seen in narratives
(Marsh, Butler, & Umanath, 2012). One study showed audiences judged science facts embedded
within a narrative to be more truthful than facts not included in the narrative (Dahlstrom, 2010).
Indeed, inaccurate facts presented in narratives are often remembered by audiences and then
misattributed to reputable sources (Barriga, Shapiro, & Fernandez, 2010; Butler, Zaromb, Lyle,
& Roediger, 2009; Marsh, Meade, & Roediger III, 2003).
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
136
Barriga et al. (2010) suggest that the tendency for people to believe facts they have read in
narratives occurs because the narrative reminds people of things they believe they heard
previously from credible sources. They write:
It is cognitively taxing for fiction readers to retrieve from memory the original source of
factual information, particularly when it is not central to the understanding of the plot. Thus,
people may remember facts introduced in a story, but do not connect them with the original
source, believing instead that they have “always known” the fact to be true. (Barriga et al.,
2010, p. 6)
These findings are significant for cli-fi scholars because they suggest cli-fi audiences and
readers may absorb inaccurate information about climate change through exposure to these
narratives. This is not to discount a multitude of other factors (such as education and scientific
literacy) that contribute to attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge about climate change, but it does
raise the possibility of cli-fi influencing audiences in ways that we are, as of yet, unaware.
The persuasive power of narrative is related to several interconnected factors, including
transportation. Green and Brock (2000, p. 701) define transportation as being “absorbed in a
story or lost in a narrative world.” When audiences feel transported, they become more open to
incorporating new facts, attitudes, and beliefs presented in the narrative into their own
worldviews (Moyer-Guse, 2008; Shen, Ahern, & Baker, 2014).
In turn, a narrative’s realism can influence how easily audiences are transported into the
world of the story (R. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2008; R. W. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009; Caputo &
Rouner, 2011; Hall, 2003). Busselle and Bilandzic (2008) suggest most audiences approach
fictional narratives with an understanding that they are consuming something that is not real and
willingly suspend disbelief in order to engage, allowing for transportation. However, when
audiences encounter aspects of the narrative that seem unrealistic, their capacity for disbelief is
challenged and transportation is interrupted (R. W. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009; Rooney, Benson,
& Hennessy, 2012). Busselle and Bilandzic suggest violations of external realism (by depicting
events that cannot or would not happen in the real world) or internal realism (by depicting events
that go against the internal logic of the narrative world or create plot holes) are especially
disruptive to transportation (2009), thus reducing the narrative’s ability to influence attitudes and
beliefs.
Realism and emotion
Research on realism suggests perceived realism and emotional involvement in films are
positively correlated (Konijn, van der Molen, & van Nes, 2009; Tan, 2008). Audiences
emotionally engage more with narratives that seem real. In turn, this engagement sustains
feelings of realism by encouraging viewers to set aside reservations about aspects of the story
that seem unrealistic.
Emotional engagement can be broken when the narrative violates internal or external realism
(R. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2008; Rooney et al., 2012). Such violations take audiences out of the
world of the narrative by reminding them that the story is “just a film” (Rooney et al., 2012, p.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
137
407). In turn, the decreased emotional involvement in the film can then erode perceptions of
realism.
Whether films can evoke emotions in audiences is important because climate change
communications scholarship suggests that the emotional content of media messages can
influence audience reactions (Beattie, Sale, & McGuire, 2011; Smith & Leiserowitz, 2012,
2014). Smith and Leiserowitz (2014), for instance, found that media that evoke hope, interest,
and worry are positively correlated with support for climate change mitigation policies.
Studies also have looked at the effect of cli-fi films on audiences’ level of emotion (Beattie et
al., 2011) and environmental concern (Howell, 2011; Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al., 2006). In
general, these studies find that fictional cli-fi films can elevate emotion levels in audiences. In
turn, this may cause audiences to feel more concern about climate change, which is then linked
with a desire to take action (Beattie et al., 2011). A key question then becomes how audiences
perceive climate change and climate science in disaster cli-fi films. Although such films portray
climate change via emotionally dramatic visual images, they are nonetheless fictional and may
lose audience involvement via unrealistic dramatizations.
Methods
This study uses qualitative methods and focus groups to understand audience perceptions of
realism and fiction in disaster cli-fi films. I draw on established focus group methodology
(Krueger & Casey, 2009; Morgan, 1993, 1996) to center analysis on the question of which
aspects of disaster cli-fi films impact audience perceptions. I used the grounded theory guidelines
laid out by Charmaz for analysis (Charmaz, 2006). These guidelines allow me to explore how
respondents create meaning from the narratives and “minimize[e] preconceived ideas about the
research project and the data” (Charmaz, 2008, p. 155).
Groups ranged from two to five respondents drawn from the student body of a university in
the southeastern United States. I recruited students through a social sciences participant pool run
by the university and participants received research course credit for their participation in the
study. My research assistant and I determined that we had reached saturation after six focus
group sessions. Specifically, we reviewed the notes and transcripts collected during each focus
group session and compared them to the notes and transcripts from previous sessions to establish
whether any new themes had arisen. We halted the focus groups after we concluded that they
were no longer producing new themes.
During each session, participants watched one of three disaster cli-fi films: NYC: Tornado
Terror (2008), F6 Twister (Christmas Twister) (2012), or Lightning: Bolts of Destruction (2003).
Four groups viewed NYC: Tornado Terror to standardize responses between groups, and I
continued using this film until I reached saturation. Because disaster cli-fi films vary
considerably in terms of their realism, I held two additional focus groups in which students were
shown either F6 Twister (Christmas Twister) or Lightning: Bolts of Destruction to ensure that
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
138
responses were not limited to NYC: Tornado Terror. Although the groups viewed a total of three
films, the themes that emerged from the F6 Twister (Christmas Twister) and Lightning: Bolts of
Destruction sessions were overwhelmingly similar to those from the NYC: Tornado Terror
groups, suggesting that audiences viewing different low-budget disaster cli-fi films generally
respond to them similarly.
I asked participants a series of open-ended questions to elicit their reactions to the films.
Questions progressed from general to specific. I first asked participants to note specific scenes or
moments within the film that impacted them and how they reacted to the films’ various
characters. I then asked participants whether the film had a message. Some groups immediately
brought up environmental themes, but I took care not to lead them to environmental messages. I
followed this with questions about the film’s realism and its depiction of science. Finally, I asked
participants whether they believed the film had impacted their own environmental attitudes and
beliefs and whether it could affect other people’s environmental attitudes and beliefs.
I analyzed the focus group results using grounded theory, beginning with transcribing the
audio files and coding the transcripts as per Charmaz (2006). I coded the transcripts for mentions
of climate change or global warming. The films tended to use “global warming” over “climate
change” and most participants echoed this. I also coded for content related to science and the
scientists in the films, as well as for statements about specific natural disasters featured in the
films. I paid particular attention to statements that conveyed skepticism, uncertainty, as well as
outright disbelief. Through analyzing the combinations of particular themes within the films as
well as attitudes expressed by the participants towards these themes, I drew several conclusions
as to how elements of disaster cli-fi films impact participants.
Results
Themes of climate change and the environment
Almost all participants cited action scenes featuring natural disasters in response to questions
about what images or moments stood out to them. In particular, they cited outlandish and
scientifically inaccurate scenes. The NYC Tornado Terror groups, for instance, frequently cited a
scene in which a ball of lightning electrocutes an office worker as she is sheltering from the
storm.
PARTICIPANT 16: [T]hat really big one [ball of lightning] that went inside the building.
PARTICIPANT 2: Probably remember the scene with the electrical ball coming into the
building.
Another commonly mentioned moment involved New York City skyscrapers illuminated by
St. Elmo’s fire. St. Elmo’s fire is a real phenomenon but was inaccurately depicted as a luminous
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
139
green substance coating the outside of buildings. The office workers use rubberized mats to
make their way down the stairwell, escaping the electrified structure.
INTERVIEWER: So, are there particular scenes…that stood out to you?
PARTICIPANT 11: Probably the stairwell with the electricity going up.
PARTICIPANT 17: The electricity throughout the film, in the stairwell and on the buildings.
You could just see the electricity [on the side of the building].
Participants were divided on the central theme of the films. Some participants saw themes of
persistence and being appreciative of what you have in life. They spoke about the persistence of
Cassie, the scientist protagonist of NYC Tornado Terror and her husband, Jim, the deputy mayor
of New York City.
PARTICIPANT 6: I guess, I mean generally, at least Cassie demonstrated persistence in the
matter. Sticking to her ultimate plan, and being persistent and so devoted
to some form of goal. And don’t give up.
PARTICIPANT 12: The one guy at the end…he made it sound like the message was appreciate
what you have.
PARTICIPANT 24: Believe in yourself.
Other participants saw climate change as a predominant theme.
PARTICIPANT 1: I think a lot of it is a message about global warming. I generally agree
with that.
PARTICIPANT 12: They’re saying that global warming is changing the environment in ways
that we can’t understand yet.
PARTICIPANT 22: It was pretty politically charged, with the whole idea of global warming
and its causing all these rapid movements is a big, I don’t know when this
movie was made, but still a pretty big politically charged film.
Several participants had reservations about climate change as the narrative’s main theme,
noting that it was only loosely tied into the overall plot.
INTERVIEWER: You mentioned global warming. Is that a major theme in this, would you
say?
PARTICIPANT 12: Yeah, they did mention it, but I feel like they would have talked more
about it if that was the main cause.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
140
Participants who also expressed skepticism about anthropogenic climate change had a strong
reaction to the narrative’s climate elements. Participant 22, for instance, called F6 Twister
(Christmas Twister) “politically charged” and argued that the film was highly inaccurate. He
cited Logan, a climate-skeptic meteorologist and one of the film’s main antagonists, as having
the most appropriate view of climate change.
PARTICIPANT 22: I think Logan was the only one who was right in saying that things happen
and [there is] a constant stage of change.
Likewise, Participant 20 eagerly identified climate change as a theme of the film NYC
Tornado Terror and described it as “propaganda.” He compared it to the episode “ManBearPig”
(Parker, 2006) from the television show South Park.
PARTICIPANT 20: I just saw it as a giant environmentalist propaganda type. Way out of the
scope. There's laser lightning beams falling from the apartment...They're
[the climate scientists] always right and the government never listens to us
and they don't care about anything. I just thought it was so out of the scope
that I couldn't really get into the characters. I just kind of was upset with
the writers and the way they, it was so overboard. I honestly thought [of]
South Park, "ManBearPig."
Here, the participant argues that NYC Tornado Terror is similar to Gore’s film An
Inconvenient Truth (2006), which attempted to warn people about global warming but was
decried by climate change deniers as alarmist. The “ManBearPig” episode of South Park shows
Gore causing destruction in his wild attempts to warn people about the fictional ManBearPig,
before announcing that he is producing a new film starring himself.
Participants who were concerned about climate change also identified environmental themes
but tended to be less emphatic that climate change was a central message, suggesting that climate
skeptics are more likely to pick up on climate change themes in pop culture than people who
support the consensus. Climate skeptics may see such media representations of climate change as
threatening their worldview (Kahan, 2012; Kahan, Jenkins-Smith, & Braman, 2011), shaping
their opinions about the narrative in question.
Relationship between climate change and disasters in film
Participants overwhelmingly questioned the connection between climate change and natural
disasters as depicted by the films. Many accepted the link between climate change and extreme
weather, but (justifiably) felt that the films did not accurately portray the link. Participants noted
that the films contained only a cursory discussion of climate change and failed to connect it with
disasters in a meaningful way. This lack of a connection with climate change was a major factor
in participants’ reluctance to identify climate change as a major theme.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
141
PARTICIPANT 2: One of the things that stood out to me was…they never gave much of an
explanation as to what was going on. The news kept saying that it was a
side effect of global warming.
PARTICIPANT 11: I mean, they mention it [climate change], but to me it doesn’t seem related
because when I think of global warming, I think “Holy crap, everyone’s
going to die.”
Some participants speculated that climate change was simply a way to advance the plot.
PARTICIPANT 14: It’s just something to blame for this. We don’t really understand it, but
let’s say it’s global warming…They had a good story, but they needed
something in the background [that was] scientific. Sort of reminded me of
“The Day After Tomorrow.”
One participant remarked that the film’s treatment of climate change was so irrelevant to the rest
of the plot that she had forgotten it by the film’s ending.
PARTICIPANT 23: Well, it’s something she mentioned, but it’s not throughout the movie and
it’s being reintroduced to you and it’s being explained to you. It’s just
briefly mentioned…and [I] totally even forgot by the end of it that they
mentioned global warming because it was just in passing.
Environmental messages that may be present in disaster cli-fi films such as this are thus diluted
by the ambiguous relationship between disasters and climate change and the lack of a concrete
mechanism linking the two.
Speed and severity of disasters
Participants expressed skepticism as to whether the natural disasters depicted could unfold with
the speed and severity depicted in the films. Several participants noted the extreme nature of the
disasters made the film seem less realistic and credible.
PARTICIPANT 21: [I]t loses all credibility with just the magnitude of how frequent these
tornadoes are…The chance of this happening is extremely low, so it loses
all its credibility when you’re pushing it to that extent.
One participant suggested the extreme and unusual disasters may lead to audiences’ dismissing
not only the film itself, but climate science as sensationalized.
PARTICIPANT 1: Well, global warming causes more extreme weather and…hurricanes or
tornadoes and things like that. I don’t think you got anything this extreme
where a tornado shoots lightning. So, people who watch would be, “Oh,
that’s not real global warming, that’s exaggerated.”
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
142
Others felt that the disasters’ rapid speed damaged the films’ realism.
PARTICIPANT 5: I feel like it happened too fast.
PARTICIPANT 12: [W]ith the science…let’s not forget that this all happened in the span of a
day.
PARTICIPANT 17: It was really drastic. “Oh, nice day,” and then the tornadoes show up and
then the storm would go about it and then it turned into completely gray
sky. It really cannot happen that fast, I'm pretty sure that it doesn't happen
in 3 minutes.
The disasters’ rapid speed added to the lack of scientific context to make the film less impactful
for one participant:
PARTICIPANT 18: I just didn't like the fact that they were…I think they [the main characters]
were at a party or something like that, and these things [the tornadoes] just
come up out of nowhere. So, can you at least tell us what led to it over the
years? To me they should have done something to lead up to that…It
scares me for a second and then it's just, “Can this possibly happen?”
Exaggerating the speed and severity of natural disasters associated with climate change appears
to damage external realism for audiences, potentially making the films less likely to impact their
attitudes and beliefs.
Representation of science in disaster cli-fi films
The depiction of science was a significant source of skepticism and uncertainty for participants.
Many participants found the science and underlying concepts unrealistic. When asked how
accurate the film’s science was, some participants outright rejected it:
PARTICIPANT 21: Literally zero. There’s nothing behind it.
PARTICIPANT 19: I think the majority of the stuff we saw in the film was really unrealistic.
Similar to Lowe et al.’s results (Lowe et al., 2006), some participants expressed uncertainty
as to the boundary between factual science and film science. Despite criticizing the films for lack
of scientific realism, comments made by several participants reveal a degree of uncertainty
regarding science fact vs. fiction. Many participants admitted they were unsure whether the
phenomena depicted—such as St. Elmo’s fire and cloud seeding—were possible. They expressed
surprise when other participants pointed out that these were not entirely fictionalized concepts,
expressing tentative acceptance of the fictionalized science.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
143
PARTICIPANT 11: I thought it was pretty cool, the idea that, with the mini tornadoes, it’s kind
of cool. I didn’t know that they get cold on the inside, though.
PARTICIPANT 14: I don’t know too much about extreme weather, so, I mean to me just the
idea that something like that could potentially happen is really crazy.
PARTICIPANT 1: [I]t’s something that could happen, hypothetically.
PARTICIPANT 3: I’m not an expert but I think anything’s possible.
Other participants were more ambivalent:
PARTICIPANT 2: Could be possible. One of the things that stood out to me was that…they
never gave much of an explanation as to what was going on. The news
kept saying that it was a side effect of global warming.
PARTICIPANT 8: I was thinking about the high-low atmosphere splitting and things like
that. I don’t know if that’s a thing, but that just sounds more
believable.…So there were certain aspects of it, you know, that I feel were
possible.
PARTICIPANT 23: I feel like lightning can definitely kill people and if it’s powerful enough,
can cause a fire or whatever, but I don’t know. Maybe small parts of it
were realistic, but most of it wasn’t.
PARTICIPANT 25: It’s just the concept in general that was interesting, that one massive storm
could overtake…I wasn’t sure. It didn’t seem really real. I don’t know in
terms of the scientific part of it, to determine if it’s actually feasible.
Several participants explained they were unqualified to judge the films’ accuracy without
further research.
PARTICIPANT 11: I don’t have anything to compare it to yet [in terms of accuracy]. I haven’t
researched it.
PARTICIPANT 18: I don't know if something like this could be a possibility. I would have to
do research on it to speak on it, but it's definitely scary if it can happen. So
[it] definitely scares people.
Many participants said the film disasters were too outlandish and vague to look up on their
own, but others expressed curiosity. In line with Hart and Leiserowitz’s findings on informationseeking
among The Day After Tomorrow audiences (Hart & Leiserowitz, 2009), participants
expressed interest in learning more about the disasters in the film. I asked participants what, if
anything, they would look up after the discussion.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
144
PARTICIPANT 12: How much of that [the tornadoes in the film] is actually true.
PARTICIPANT 15: Are there any tornados [such as those shown] that actually happened?
Participants also critiqued the films’ representation of science, particularly the lone scientist
trope. The mysterious disasters in all three films are eventually understood and/or destroyed by a
single scientist character who races the clock with his/her research. Participants correctly argue
that science is frequently a collaborative enterprise, making it unlikely that a lone scientist could
successfully solve such a complex problem on his/her own.
PARTICIPANT 4: I think there would be a lot more people involved, a lot more.
PARTICIPANT 16: She was the only one who came up with something. I guess she was the
head and she was the smartest one…But I mean, I was wondering, “What
were those people [the rest of the scientific staff] doing?”
Likewise, participants criticized the speed with which the lead scientist identified the
problem and developed a solution. These critiques were often related to participants’ perceptions
that the disaster evolved too quickly:
PARTICIPANT 11: Science does not take a day.
PARTICIPANT 12: [T]here would be a lot more testing and stuff going on, especially if it is
some kind of disaster…But they were so sure – at least she was – so sure
that this is exactly the perfect solution, and you don’t get that sort of
certainty in that short of time.
PARTICIPANT 11: And remember two hours before [revealing the solution to the tornado
outbreak] she [the lead scientist] was like, “This will take years.” Two
seconds later – done. No.
Efficacy and disaster cli-fi films
One important criticism participants had of the film was that it did not contain any suggestions as
to how to combat anthropogenic climate change in the real world. Participants felt the
environmental message of the film was weakened by telling them a story about the dangers of
climate change but failing to provide any suggestions for concrete actions audience members
could take in their own lives to mitigate the threat.
PARTICIPANT 5: I think this film in particular didn’t really address that [taking action to
minimize the threat] because they didn’t give an explanation for why it
was out there. They just jumped right in there. There wasn’t, “Oh there’s
so much greenhouse gas,” or “Oh you’re using too much fossil fuels.”
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
145
PARTICIPANT 14: [T]hey didn’t really go deeper in terms of this is why it’s global warming
specifically…And [there was] no message about, “We need to fix what
we’re doing or something.” They’re just, “Oh let’s stop this, what’s going
on now.”
PARTICIPANT 8: I feel like it doesn’t say any of the techniques [to stop climate change] – if
someone was to, you know, think it was their fault.
This criticism is in line with other studies in the social sciences that argue the lack of concrete
suggestions for how to combat climate change weakens environmental messages (Hart &
Feldman, 2014; Li, 2014).
Impacts on environmental attitudes
The consensus was that these films are not particularly effective at changing environmental
attitudes, particularly in light of the criticisms regarding lack of efficacy. Although some
participants suggested that less educated, less informed members of the public would be more
likely to take the films at face value, participants generally felt that the films were too inaccurate
to impact attitudes. Some participants even went as far as to say films such as the ones used in
the study made people less likely to accept anthropogenic climate change as a real threat for
which action is required.
PARTICIPANT 2: I think if people would see this movie they would kind of think that global
warming is not real because of the movie.
PARTICIPANT 13: I mean from this [film], they probably think of it as a joke.
That being said, participants did not outright reject the idea that fictional films could have
indirect impacts on attitudes. Even though viewers might dismiss the films as unrealistic, a
lingering uncertainty could remain, as some participants noted:
PARTICIPANT 4: I think that people might not believe it, but then still have the doubt that it
could happen.
PARTICIPANT 2: I think in the back of their mind.
Because participants had difficulty distinguishing where fact and fiction diverged, the
possibility of extreme disasters seemed to remain for some participants.
PARTICIPANT 18: [O]f course you're going to have people that say, “Oh Syfy, just another
one of those movies.” Then you have some people that at first hand, will
get kind of scared, because they're like, “Well, I don't know much about
science,” like myself, and they'll think, “Well, maybe this possibly can
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
146
happen, even though it's a movie.” But for a while people might think,
“Maybe this can happen.”
Discussion
Several conclusions can be drawn from these results. Participants generally noticed recurring
mentions of climate change but were divided on whether these constituted a major theme. Much
of this hesitancy came from the lack of a detailed, specific discussion of climate change within
the films. Interestingly, the few participants with a skeptical attitude toward anthropogenic
climate change tended to be most adamant that climate change was a theme. It may be that such
participants were not sufficiently transported into the world of the narrative to preclude back
arguing against the films’ premises. It may also be that participants who have strong feelings of
climate skepticism may have picked up on climate change references early on because they
perceived such references as an effort to challenge their deeply-held beliefs.
The predominant emotional responses to the films were incredulity and confusion.
Underneath this umbrella of skepticism were three specific areas of disbelief: the causal
relationship between climate change and disasters (violation of internal realism), the swiftness
and severity of the portrayed disasters (violation of external realism), and the representation of
science (violation of external realism).
Despite their generally skeptical attitude toward the depiction of science and climate change
in the films, participants remained uncertain about the barrier between science fact and science
fiction. Even though participants openly expressed disbelief that the events in the film could
happen, many seemed unsure as to the degree to which the films exaggerated and invented
phenomena. This uncertainty suggests participants struggled to pinpoint the specific point at
which the films departed from established science, similar to the results found by Lowe et al.
(2006).
It appears the violations of internal and external realism documented impacted audience
involvement in the films, as predicted by the literature. The combination of extreme,
scientifically-questionable events and weak causation in disaster cli-fi films caused audiences to
lose track of the climate change theme. In addition, participants questioned the sped-up timelines
and exaggerated intensity of the disasters in the films. Unrealistic timelines and impossible
impacts from climate change are common criticisms of disaster cli-fi films levied by both
filmgoers and scientists alike (Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al., 2006). Participants felt the use of a
lone scientist trope, where a single individual diagnoses and solves the disaster on his/her own,
was inaccurate. They suggested these features made the film less realistic and weakened their
engagement with the storyline. This is in line with other research which suggests that films must
maintain a certain level of believability to ensure that audiences remain engaged (Lowe et al.,
2006).
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
147
Overall, participants doubted the films’ ability to affect environmental attitudes and beliefs.
They argued the films were too inaccurate and lacking in credibility to influence how the public
thinks about climate change. These findings contradict other research on environmental attitudes
and fictional films. Although the participants dismissed the influence of these particular films on
environmental attitudes, studies have shown that The Day After Tomorrow (2004) did affect
audiences’ attitudes toward climate change (Howell, 2011; Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al.,
2006).
There are a few possible explanations for the disconnect between what participants say
about the impacts of film on environmental attitudes and what researchers have measured. The
third-person effect is a well-documented phenomenon within communications studies (Davison,
1983) which “predicts that individuals will perceive media messages to have greater effects on
other people than on themselves” (Salwen & Dupagne, 1999, p. 523). The third-person effect is
thought to arise because people generally believe they are smarter than others; thus, they are less
likely to be persuaded by the media than others who are less intelligent (Paul, Salwen, &
Dupagne, 2000). Given the participants’ observation that the natural disasters depicted in disaster
cli-fi films are highly unscientific and exaggerated, it could be that participants assumed that no
one was gullible enough to be influenced by them. Theoretically, this would suggest a sort of
floor to the third-person effect whereby some forms of media are seen as so obviously absurd
that no one could be influenced by them—even the assumed less-intelligent third-person.
I suggest that what is more likely is that participants were more influenced by the films than
they openly acknowledged. Indeed, given the uncertainty expressed by many participants as to
where scientific fact stopped and science fiction began, the films may have impacted participant
environmental attitudes without their explicit awareness. This impact would support the findings
of Lowe et al. (2006) and Leiserowitz (2004), as well as the concerns that audiences will mistake
fictional science for science facts (Kirby, 2003b, 2003a). Studies that suggest people struggle to
separate fact from fiction when watching science fiction films (Barriga et al., 2010) and
historical fiction films (Butler et al., 2009; Marsh et al., 2012, 2003) add to this fear.
Thus, it is worth considering the possibility that, although fictional films are unlikely to
sway climate deniers and unlikely to shake supporters of the consensus, subtler environmental
beliefs and scientific understandings of issues like climate change could be affected. These
misunderstandings have implications for climate change communicators and advocates, who
may find themselves spending more time debunking misperceptions. Although more research is
needed, it is possible that the inaccurate science in these films can contribute to a general lack of
understanding of what causes climate change, how humans are contributing, and why climate
change is a threat.
Equally concerning is the possibility that film audiences, having been exposed to inaccurate
ideas about climate change and storms in disaster cli-fi, may be more susceptible to
misinformation in the event of actual storms. During the preparations for Hurricane Irma in
September 2017, for example, an image of a fake news ticker with the caption “Irma Now
Contains Sharks” went viral on social media (Porter, 2017). Irma was the subject of a variety of
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
148
hoaxes, including the rumor that it would make landfall in Florida as a Category 6 storm
(hurricanes are measured on the Saffir-Simpson scale, which goes up to 5) (Ohlheiser, 2017). It
may be that people who saw these films misremembered the inaccurate, fictional storm
information later, making them more receptive to misleading claims during an actual extreme
weather event. More research is needed to determine to what extent, if any, inaccurate
information in cli-fi influences perceptions of real disasters. However, as was evident during
Irma, debunking such misinformation falls on the shoulders of meteorologists and other public
safety officials whose time would be better spent warning of actual dangers.
Limitations and directions for future research
Using undergraduate students for focus groups poses certain limitations, as these students are not
representative of movie audiences in the United States or elsewhere. It is also possible younger
media consumers have different levels of scientific education and understanding than older
media consumers, impacting what they see as realistic or unrealistic. Further studies should
explore how a wider audience interacts with disaster cli-fi films to see whether these results hold
true for other demographics.
Audiences exposed to historical fiction films often incorrectly interpret historical
fabrications as fact (Butler et al., 2009; Marsh et al., 2012, 2003). Whether such
misinterpretations occur with disaster cli-fi films as well is an area of study that would be quite
fruitful and important. Specifically, if audiences are absorbing misleading or false information
about climate change from popular culture, science communicators may be forced to devote
increasing amounts of time and energy to debunking false notions propagated in fiction.
Other studies exploring the influence of film on environmental attitudes have suggested
films can affect attitudes and beliefs about climate change (Howell, 2011; Leiserowitz, 2004;
Lowe et al., 2006). These studies also suggest fictional representations of climate change in film
can influence audiences’ intended actions on climate change. Because of the limited scope of
this study, it is not possible to say whether the low-budget disaster cli-fi films such as the ones
examined here have similar effects on audience actions. Given the proliferation of these films in
the media marketplace, it would be useful for science communicators to understand how these
films impact their audiences’ willingness to act on climate change.
Although it has been over a decade since the theatrical release of The Day After Tomorrow
(2004), new examples of climate change in popular culture are multiplying rapidly. The
Sharknado series—which specifically invokes climate change—has become a cult classic, with
five films total in the series as well as books, documentaries, and a plethora of merchandise. Clifi
is rapidly becoming an important genre in film and other media. Social scientists who wish to
study public attitudes on climate change will increasingly need to engage with pop culture. My
findings suggest the relationship between pop culture and attitudes is complex but specific
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
149
components of film and fiction may be more important than others in determining audience
reactions.
References
Barriga, C. A., Shapiro, M. A., & Fernandez, M. L. (2010). Science information in fictional
movies: Effects of context and gender. Science Communication, 32(1), 3–24.
doi:10.1177/1075547009340338
Beattie, G., Sale, L., & McGuire, L. (2011). An inconvenient truth? Can a film really affect
psychological mood and our explicit attitudes towards climate change? Semiotica, 187(1–4),
105–125. doi:10.1515/semi.2011.066
Bloom, D. (2017). The Cli-Fi Report. Retrieved from http://cli-fi.net/
Boykoff, J. M., & Boykoff, M. T. (2007). Climate change and journalistic norms: A case-study
of US mass-media coverage. Geoforum, 38(6), 1190–1204.
doi:10.1016/j.geoforum.2007.01.008
Boykoff, M. T., & Rajan, S. R. (2007). Signals and noise. Mass-media coverage of climate
change in the USA and the UK. EMBO Reports, 8(3), 207–211.
doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400924
Braddock, K., & Dillard, J. P. (2016). Meta-analytic evidence for the persuasive effect of
narratives on beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Communication Monographs,
83(4), 446–467. doi:10.1080/03637751.2015.1128555
Braddock, K., & Horgan, J. (2016). Towards a guide for constructing and disseminating
counternarratives to reduce support for terrorism. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 39(5),
381–404. doi:10.1080/1057610X.2015.1116277
Brulle, R. J., Carmichael, J., & Jenkins, J. C. (2012). Shifting public opinion on climate change:
An empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the U.S.,
2002-2010. Climatic Change, 114(1), 169. doi:10.1007/s10584-012-0403-y
Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories:
A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18(2), 255–
280. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00322.x
Busselle, R. W., & Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychology,
12(4), 321–347. doi:10.1080/15213260903287259
Butler, A. C., Zaromb, F. M., Lyle, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Using popular films to
enhance classroom learning: The good, the bad, and the interesting. Psychological Science,
20(9), 1161–1168. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02410.x
Caputo, N. M., & Rouner, D. (2011). Narrative processing of entertainment media and mental
illness stigma. Health Communication, 26(7), 595–604. doi:10.1080/10410236.2011.560787
Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative
analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
150
Charmaz, K. (2008). Grounded theory as an emergent method. In S. N. Hesse-Biber & P. Leavy
(Eds.), Handbook of emergent methods. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Corbett, J., & Durfee, J. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science: Media representations of
global warming. Science Communication, 26(2), 129–151. doi:10.1177/1075547004270234
Dahlstrom, M. F. (2010). The role of causality in information acceptance in narratives: An
example from science communication. Communication Research, 37(6), 857–875.
doi:10.1177/0093650210362683
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. The Public Opinion
Quarterly, 47(1), 1–15. doi:10.1086/268763
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public
narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701–721.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.701
Hall, A. (2003). Reading realism: Audiences’ evaluations of the reality of media texts. Journal of
Communication, 53(4), 624–641. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb02914.x
Hart, P. S., & Leiserowitz, A. (2009). Finding the teachable moment: An analysis of
information-seeking behavior on global warming related websites during the release of The
Day After Tomorrow. Environmental Communication - A Journal of Nature and Culture,
3(3), 355–366. doi:10.1080/17524030903265823
Howell, R. A. (2011). Lights, camera...action? Altered attitudes and behaviour in response to the
climate change film The Age of Stupid. Global Environmental Change, 21(1), 177–187.
doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.09.004
Kahan, D. (2012). Why we are poles apart on climate change: The problem isn’t the public’s
reasoning capacity; it’s the polluted science-communication environment that drives people
apart. Nature, 488(7411), 255. doi:10.1038/488255a
Kahan, D., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus.
Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174. doi:10.1080/13669877.2010.511246
Kaplan, E. A. (2015). Climate trauma: Foreseeing the future in dystopian film and fiction. New
Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Kirby, D. A. (2003a). Science consultants, fictional films, and scientific practice. Social Studies
of Science, 33(2), 231–268. doi:10.1177/03063127030332015
Kirby, D. A. (2003b). Scientists on the set: Science consultants and the communication of
science in visual fiction. Public Understanding of Science, 12(3), 261–278.
doi:10.1177/0963662503123005
Konijn, E. A., van der Molen, J. H. W., & van Nes, S. (2009). Emotions bias perceptions of
realism in audiovisual media: Why we may take fiction for real. Discourse Processes, 46(4),
309–340. doi:10.1080/01638530902728546
Krueger, R. A., & Casey, M. A. (2009). Focus groups: A practical guide for applied research.
Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications.
Leiserowitz, A. (2004). Before and after the day after tomorrow: A US study of climate change
risk perception. Environment, 46(9), 22–37.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
151
Lowe, T., Brown, K., Dessai, S., de Franca Doria, M., Haynes, K., & Vincent, K. (2006). Does
tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public perceptions of climate change. Public
Understanding of Science, 15(4), 435–457. doi:10.1177/0963662506063796
Marsh, E. J., Butler, A. C., & Umanath, S. (2012). Using fictional sources in the classroom:
Applications from cognitive psychology. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 449–469.
doi:10.1007/s10648-012-9204-0
Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger III, H. L. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of
Memory and Language, 49(4), 519–536. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00092-5
Merchant, B. (2013). Behold the Rise of Dystopian “Cli-Fi.” Retrieved fromhttps://motherboard.vice.com/en_us/article/ypp7nj/behold-the-rise-of-cli-fi
Morgan, D. L. (1993). Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art. Newbury Park,
CA: Sage Publications.
Morgan, D. L. (1996). Focus groups. Annual Review of Sociology, 22(1), 129–152.
doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.22.1.129
Moyer-Guse, E. (2008). Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: Explaining the persuasive
effects of entertainment-education messages. Communication Theory, 18(3), 407–425.
doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00328.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2011). An experimental test of the effects of fictional framing on
attitudes. Social Science Quarterly, 92(1), 79–99. doi:10.1111/j.1540-6237.2011.00758.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2013). The implications of fictional media for political beliefs.
American Politics Research, 41(1), 122–146. doi:10.1177/1532673X12453758
Murphy, P. (2014). Directing the weather, producing the climate. In Alex, R.S., Deborah, S.S., &
Sachindev, P.S. (Eds.), Culture and media: Ecocritical explorations. Newcastle upon Tyne,
UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Murray, R. L., & Heumann, J. K. (2009). Ecology and popular film: Cinema on the edge.
Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Ohlheiser, A. (2017, September 11). Analysis | A running list of viral hoaxes about Irma —
including one shared by the White House. The Washington Post Retrieved fromhttps://www.washingtonpost.com
Parker, T. (2006). ManBearPig. Comedy Central. Retrieved from http://southpark.cc.com/fullepisodes/
s10e06-manbearpig
Paul, B., Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (2000). The third-person effect: A meta-analysis of the
perceptual hypothesis. Mass Communication and Society, 3(1), 57–85.
doi:10.1207/S15327825MCS0301_04
Porter, T. (2017, September 9). Here’s some of the fake news and false rumors about Hurricane
Irma. Retrieved from http://www.newsweek.com/hurricane-irma-fake-news-false-rumorsspread-
storm-662419
Rooney, B., Benson, C., & Hennessy, E. (2012). The apparent reality of movies and emotional
arousal: A study using physiological and self-report measures. Poetics, 40, 405–422.
doi:10.1016/j.poetic.2012.07.004
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
152
Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (1999). The third-person effect. Communication Research, 26(5),
523. doi: 10.1177/009365099026005001
Shen, F., Ahern, L., & Baker, M. (2014). Stories that count: Influence of news narratives on
issue attitudes. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 91(1), 98–117.
doi:10.1177/1077699013514414
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). The rise of global warming skepticism: Exploring affective
image associations in the United States over time. Risk Analysis, 32(6), 1021–1032.
doi:10.1605/01.301-0019863810.2012
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2014). The role of emotion in global warming policy support and
opposition. Risk Analysis, 34(5), 937–948. doi:10.1111/risa.12140
Svoboda, M. (2014). The Long Melt: The Lingering Influence of The Day After Tomorrow.
Retrieved from http://www.yaleclimateconnections.org/2014/11/the-long-melt-thelingering-
influence-of-the-day-after-tomorrow/
Svoboda, M. (2016). Cli-fi on the screen(s): Patterns in the representations of climate change in
fictional films. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 7(1), 43–64.
doi:10.1002/wcc.381
Tan, E. S. H. (2008). Entertainment is emotion: The functional architecture of the entertainment
experience. Media Psychology, 11(1), 28–51. doi:10.1080/15213260701853161
Introduction
The mass media are a crucial source of information regarding the causes and predicted effects of
anthropogenic climate change for many people (J. M. Boykoff & Boykoff, 2007; M. T. Boykoff
& Rajan, 2007; Brulle, Carmichael, & Jenkins, 2012; Corbett & Durfee, 2004). As a result,
researchers have begun to analyze the discourse surrounding climate change in the media,
particularly the news media. Although these studies have produced valuable information, they do
not address the effects of non-news media—namely, fictional narratives—on climate change
attitudes and beliefs.
Audience Reactions to Climate Change and Science in
Disaster Cli-fi Films: A Qualitative Analysis
Lauren N. Griffin
University of Florida
Article Information
Received: July 17, 2017
Accepted: October 8, 2017
Published online: December 20, 2017
Abstract
Little scholarly attention has been paid to how audiences
interpret pop culture messages about climate. This paper
addresses this issue by taking up the case of disaster cli-fi
films and exploring how audiences react to film
representations of climate change. It draws on data from
focus groups to evaluate audience responses to disaster
cli-fi films. Analysis reveals that by only briefly
discussing climate change in their plotlines, the films
weaken their environmental message. The paper
concludes with a discussion of the effects of disaster cli-fi
films on environmental attitudes and suggestions for
further research.
Journal of Public Interest Communications
ISSN (online): 2573-4342
Journal homepage: http://journals.fcla.edu/jpic/
Keywords
Climate change
Narratives
Mass media
Public understanding of science
Focus Groups
*Please send correspondences about this article to Lauren N. Griffin, College of Journalism and
Communications, University of Florida. E-mail: lngriffin@ufl.edu. Copyright Griffin 2017. This work is
licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-Non Commercial 3.0 United States License. To view a copy of
this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/us/orsend a letter to Creative Commons, PO
Box 1866, Mountain View, CA 94042, USA.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
134
Climate fiction, or “cli-fi,” (Bloom, 2017) is an emerging area of exploration for climate
change communications. Cli-fi is a growing presence in literature, cinema, and gaming and
consists of narratives that examine the causes, effects, and implications of anthropogenic climate
change (Merchant, 2013). Braddock and Horgan describe narratives as “any cohesive and
coherent account of events with an identifiable beginning, middle, and end about characters
engaged in actions that result in questions or conflicts for which answers or resolutions are
provided” (2016, pp. 382–383).
Researchers have begun to address cli-fi’s impact on public attitudes and beliefs about
climate change (Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe, Brown, Dessai, de Franca, Doria, Haynes, & Vincent,
2006; Svoboda, 2014, 2016). However, so far these approaches have mainly used quantitative
measures to examine audience responses to cli-fi. A numerical approach leaves the possibility
open that significant aspects of the interpretive process are missing from the current literature.
This paper addresses this gap by undertaking a qualitative examination of how audiences
interpret climate fiction using the subject of disaster cli-fi films.
I provide background by briefly describing cli-fi and how it relates to disaster films. I then
discuss narrative persuasion and how narratives have been used to communicate facts. I then
provide an overview of the challenge of communicating science to the public, followed by a
review of the connection between realism and emotion in driving narrative transportation. I then
review the methods and data for the study and discuss the significant impacts of film on audience
emotions, weighing this against the question of realism in film. I suggest that this question of
balance is important for understanding audience responses to climate disaster films as they
inform audience views on climate change.
I use data from focus groups of disaster cli-fi film audiences to qualitatively evaluate their
responses to climate change and climate science in three films: NYC Tornado Terror (2008),
Lightning: Bolts of Destruction (2003), and F6 Twister (also released under the title Christmas
Twister) (2012). Discussions centered on audiences’ emotional responses to the films and their
evaluation of the films’ factual accuracy and were analyzed using grounded theory. My analysis
suggests that films weaken any environmental message present by only discussing climate
change in a cursory way. My analysis also revealed audience skepticism about the speed and
severity of climate disasters depicted in the films and the problem of the lone scientist. I
conclude with a discussion of the potential effects of disaster cli-fi films on environmental
attitudes and suggestions for further research.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
135
Background
What is cli-fi?
Cli-fi is a portmanteau of the words climate and fiction coined by Dan Bloom (Bloom, 2017).
Climate change and other environmental problems are increasingly featured in fictional films
(Murray & Heumann, 2009) such as the blockbusters The Day After Tomorrow (2006) and
Snowpiercer (2013) and lower-budget productions such as Category 6 (2004). The cli-fi film
genre is broad and contains films that touch on climate change only briefly (Svoboda, 2016).
Most research on cli-fi films examines better-known productions such as The Day After
Tomorrow (Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al., 2006; Svoboda, 2014) or The Age of Stupid (Howell,
2011). Kaplan explores the broader genre and concludes cli-fi is evidence of environmental pretrauma,
or anxiety about things that will happen in the future (2015). Svoboda examines the
history of cli-fi films and begins the process of classifying these films by the type of disaster they
feature (Svoboda, 2016).
The intersection between disaster films and cli-fi
I focus here on disaster cli-fi films, a type of cli-fi that intersects with the disaster film genre. By
definition, cli-fi narratives discuss climate change, but they do not always depict natural
disasters. Mad Max: Fury Road (2015) implies climate change contributed to the collapse of
ecosystems but does not show this happening in the film itself. Disaster films focus on disasters
as they happen but do not always mention climate change. Twister (1996) is driven by storm
chasers following destructive tornadoes but does not feature climate change. Disaster cli-fi films,
on the other hand, show climate change-driven disasters as they unfold. These films frequently
depict climate change inaccurately—compressing long-term environmental changes into a few
hours, for instance (Murphy, 2014)—but they relate natural disasters to climate change to a
greater or lesser degree.
The persuasiveness of narratives
Communication scholarship suggests narratives can influence attitudes and beliefs of audiences
(Braddock & Dillard, 2016; Mulligan & Habel, 2011, 2013). Facts—including science facts—
presented in narratives may appear more real to audiences than facts not seen in narratives
(Marsh, Butler, & Umanath, 2012). One study showed audiences judged science facts embedded
within a narrative to be more truthful than facts not included in the narrative (Dahlstrom, 2010).
Indeed, inaccurate facts presented in narratives are often remembered by audiences and then
misattributed to reputable sources (Barriga, Shapiro, & Fernandez, 2010; Butler, Zaromb, Lyle,
& Roediger, 2009; Marsh, Meade, & Roediger III, 2003).
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
136
Barriga et al. (2010) suggest that the tendency for people to believe facts they have read in
narratives occurs because the narrative reminds people of things they believe they heard
previously from credible sources. They write:
It is cognitively taxing for fiction readers to retrieve from memory the original source of
factual information, particularly when it is not central to the understanding of the plot. Thus,
people may remember facts introduced in a story, but do not connect them with the original
source, believing instead that they have “always known” the fact to be true. (Barriga et al.,
2010, p. 6)
These findings are significant for cli-fi scholars because they suggest cli-fi audiences and
readers may absorb inaccurate information about climate change through exposure to these
narratives. This is not to discount a multitude of other factors (such as education and scientific
literacy) that contribute to attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge about climate change, but it does
raise the possibility of cli-fi influencing audiences in ways that we are, as of yet, unaware.
The persuasive power of narrative is related to several interconnected factors, including
transportation. Green and Brock (2000, p. 701) define transportation as being “absorbed in a
story or lost in a narrative world.” When audiences feel transported, they become more open to
incorporating new facts, attitudes, and beliefs presented in the narrative into their own
worldviews (Moyer-Guse, 2008; Shen, Ahern, & Baker, 2014).
In turn, a narrative’s realism can influence how easily audiences are transported into the
world of the story (R. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2008; R. W. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009; Caputo &
Rouner, 2011; Hall, 2003). Busselle and Bilandzic (2008) suggest most audiences approach
fictional narratives with an understanding that they are consuming something that is not real and
willingly suspend disbelief in order to engage, allowing for transportation. However, when
audiences encounter aspects of the narrative that seem unrealistic, their capacity for disbelief is
challenged and transportation is interrupted (R. W. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009; Rooney, Benson,
& Hennessy, 2012). Busselle and Bilandzic suggest violations of external realism (by depicting
events that cannot or would not happen in the real world) or internal realism (by depicting events
that go against the internal logic of the narrative world or create plot holes) are especially
disruptive to transportation (2009), thus reducing the narrative’s ability to influence attitudes and
beliefs.
Realism and emotion
Research on realism suggests perceived realism and emotional involvement in films are
positively correlated (Konijn, van der Molen, & van Nes, 2009; Tan, 2008). Audiences
emotionally engage more with narratives that seem real. In turn, this engagement sustains
feelings of realism by encouraging viewers to set aside reservations about aspects of the story
that seem unrealistic.
Emotional engagement can be broken when the narrative violates internal or external realism
(R. Busselle & Bilandzic, 2008; Rooney et al., 2012). Such violations take audiences out of the
world of the narrative by reminding them that the story is “just a film” (Rooney et al., 2012, p.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
137
407). In turn, the decreased emotional involvement in the film can then erode perceptions of
realism.
Whether films can evoke emotions in audiences is important because climate change
communications scholarship suggests that the emotional content of media messages can
influence audience reactions (Beattie, Sale, & McGuire, 2011; Smith & Leiserowitz, 2012,
2014). Smith and Leiserowitz (2014), for instance, found that media that evoke hope, interest,
and worry are positively correlated with support for climate change mitigation policies.
Studies also have looked at the effect of cli-fi films on audiences’ level of emotion (Beattie et
al., 2011) and environmental concern (Howell, 2011; Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al., 2006). In
general, these studies find that fictional cli-fi films can elevate emotion levels in audiences. In
turn, this may cause audiences to feel more concern about climate change, which is then linked
with a desire to take action (Beattie et al., 2011). A key question then becomes how audiences
perceive climate change and climate science in disaster cli-fi films. Although such films portray
climate change via emotionally dramatic visual images, they are nonetheless fictional and may
lose audience involvement via unrealistic dramatizations.
Methods
This study uses qualitative methods and focus groups to understand audience perceptions of
realism and fiction in disaster cli-fi films. I draw on established focus group methodology
(Krueger & Casey, 2009; Morgan, 1993, 1996) to center analysis on the question of which
aspects of disaster cli-fi films impact audience perceptions. I used the grounded theory guidelines
laid out by Charmaz for analysis (Charmaz, 2006). These guidelines allow me to explore how
respondents create meaning from the narratives and “minimize[e] preconceived ideas about the
research project and the data” (Charmaz, 2008, p. 155).
Groups ranged from two to five respondents drawn from the student body of a university in
the southeastern United States. I recruited students through a social sciences participant pool run
by the university and participants received research course credit for their participation in the
study. My research assistant and I determined that we had reached saturation after six focus
group sessions. Specifically, we reviewed the notes and transcripts collected during each focus
group session and compared them to the notes and transcripts from previous sessions to establish
whether any new themes had arisen. We halted the focus groups after we concluded that they
were no longer producing new themes.
During each session, participants watched one of three disaster cli-fi films: NYC: Tornado
Terror (2008), F6 Twister (Christmas Twister) (2012), or Lightning: Bolts of Destruction (2003).
Four groups viewed NYC: Tornado Terror to standardize responses between groups, and I
continued using this film until I reached saturation. Because disaster cli-fi films vary
considerably in terms of their realism, I held two additional focus groups in which students were
shown either F6 Twister (Christmas Twister) or Lightning: Bolts of Destruction to ensure that
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
138
responses were not limited to NYC: Tornado Terror. Although the groups viewed a total of three
films, the themes that emerged from the F6 Twister (Christmas Twister) and Lightning: Bolts of
Destruction sessions were overwhelmingly similar to those from the NYC: Tornado Terror
groups, suggesting that audiences viewing different low-budget disaster cli-fi films generally
respond to them similarly.
I asked participants a series of open-ended questions to elicit their reactions to the films.
Questions progressed from general to specific. I first asked participants to note specific scenes or
moments within the film that impacted them and how they reacted to the films’ various
characters. I then asked participants whether the film had a message. Some groups immediately
brought up environmental themes, but I took care not to lead them to environmental messages. I
followed this with questions about the film’s realism and its depiction of science. Finally, I asked
participants whether they believed the film had impacted their own environmental attitudes and
beliefs and whether it could affect other people’s environmental attitudes and beliefs.
I analyzed the focus group results using grounded theory, beginning with transcribing the
audio files and coding the transcripts as per Charmaz (2006). I coded the transcripts for mentions
of climate change or global warming. The films tended to use “global warming” over “climate
change” and most participants echoed this. I also coded for content related to science and the
scientists in the films, as well as for statements about specific natural disasters featured in the
films. I paid particular attention to statements that conveyed skepticism, uncertainty, as well as
outright disbelief. Through analyzing the combinations of particular themes within the films as
well as attitudes expressed by the participants towards these themes, I drew several conclusions
as to how elements of disaster cli-fi films impact participants.
Results
Themes of climate change and the environment
Almost all participants cited action scenes featuring natural disasters in response to questions
about what images or moments stood out to them. In particular, they cited outlandish and
scientifically inaccurate scenes. The NYC Tornado Terror groups, for instance, frequently cited a
scene in which a ball of lightning electrocutes an office worker as she is sheltering from the
storm.
PARTICIPANT 16: [T]hat really big one [ball of lightning] that went inside the building.
PARTICIPANT 2: Probably remember the scene with the electrical ball coming into the
building.
Another commonly mentioned moment involved New York City skyscrapers illuminated by
St. Elmo’s fire. St. Elmo’s fire is a real phenomenon but was inaccurately depicted as a luminous
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
139
green substance coating the outside of buildings. The office workers use rubberized mats to
make their way down the stairwell, escaping the electrified structure.
INTERVIEWER: So, are there particular scenes…that stood out to you?
PARTICIPANT 11: Probably the stairwell with the electricity going up.
PARTICIPANT 17: The electricity throughout the film, in the stairwell and on the buildings.
You could just see the electricity [on the side of the building].
Participants were divided on the central theme of the films. Some participants saw themes of
persistence and being appreciative of what you have in life. They spoke about the persistence of
Cassie, the scientist protagonist of NYC Tornado Terror and her husband, Jim, the deputy mayor
of New York City.
PARTICIPANT 6: I guess, I mean generally, at least Cassie demonstrated persistence in the
matter. Sticking to her ultimate plan, and being persistent and so devoted
to some form of goal. And don’t give up.
PARTICIPANT 12: The one guy at the end…he made it sound like the message was appreciate
what you have.
PARTICIPANT 24: Believe in yourself.
Other participants saw climate change as a predominant theme.
PARTICIPANT 1: I think a lot of it is a message about global warming. I generally agree
with that.
PARTICIPANT 12: They’re saying that global warming is changing the environment in ways
that we can’t understand yet.
PARTICIPANT 22: It was pretty politically charged, with the whole idea of global warming
and its causing all these rapid movements is a big, I don’t know when this
movie was made, but still a pretty big politically charged film.
Several participants had reservations about climate change as the narrative’s main theme,
noting that it was only loosely tied into the overall plot.
INTERVIEWER: You mentioned global warming. Is that a major theme in this, would you
say?
PARTICIPANT 12: Yeah, they did mention it, but I feel like they would have talked more
about it if that was the main cause.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
140
Participants who also expressed skepticism about anthropogenic climate change had a strong
reaction to the narrative’s climate elements. Participant 22, for instance, called F6 Twister
(Christmas Twister) “politically charged” and argued that the film was highly inaccurate. He
cited Logan, a climate-skeptic meteorologist and one of the film’s main antagonists, as having
the most appropriate view of climate change.
PARTICIPANT 22: I think Logan was the only one who was right in saying that things happen
and [there is] a constant stage of change.
Likewise, Participant 20 eagerly identified climate change as a theme of the film NYC
Tornado Terror and described it as “propaganda.” He compared it to the episode “ManBearPig”
(Parker, 2006) from the television show South Park.
PARTICIPANT 20: I just saw it as a giant environmentalist propaganda type. Way out of the
scope. There's laser lightning beams falling from the apartment...They're
[the climate scientists] always right and the government never listens to us
and they don't care about anything. I just thought it was so out of the scope
that I couldn't really get into the characters. I just kind of was upset with
the writers and the way they, it was so overboard. I honestly thought [of]
South Park, "ManBearPig."
Here, the participant argues that NYC Tornado Terror is similar to Gore’s film An
Inconvenient Truth (2006), which attempted to warn people about global warming but was
decried by climate change deniers as alarmist. The “ManBearPig” episode of South Park shows
Gore causing destruction in his wild attempts to warn people about the fictional ManBearPig,
before announcing that he is producing a new film starring himself.
Participants who were concerned about climate change also identified environmental themes
but tended to be less emphatic that climate change was a central message, suggesting that climate
skeptics are more likely to pick up on climate change themes in pop culture than people who
support the consensus. Climate skeptics may see such media representations of climate change as
threatening their worldview (Kahan, 2012; Kahan, Jenkins-Smith, & Braman, 2011), shaping
their opinions about the narrative in question.
Relationship between climate change and disasters in film
Participants overwhelmingly questioned the connection between climate change and natural
disasters as depicted by the films. Many accepted the link between climate change and extreme
weather, but (justifiably) felt that the films did not accurately portray the link. Participants noted
that the films contained only a cursory discussion of climate change and failed to connect it with
disasters in a meaningful way. This lack of a connection with climate change was a major factor
in participants’ reluctance to identify climate change as a major theme.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
141
PARTICIPANT 2: One of the things that stood out to me was…they never gave much of an
explanation as to what was going on. The news kept saying that it was a
side effect of global warming.
PARTICIPANT 11: I mean, they mention it [climate change], but to me it doesn’t seem related
because when I think of global warming, I think “Holy crap, everyone’s
going to die.”
Some participants speculated that climate change was simply a way to advance the plot.
PARTICIPANT 14: It’s just something to blame for this. We don’t really understand it, but
let’s say it’s global warming…They had a good story, but they needed
something in the background [that was] scientific. Sort of reminded me of
“The Day After Tomorrow.”
One participant remarked that the film’s treatment of climate change was so irrelevant to the rest
of the plot that she had forgotten it by the film’s ending.
PARTICIPANT 23: Well, it’s something she mentioned, but it’s not throughout the movie and
it’s being reintroduced to you and it’s being explained to you. It’s just
briefly mentioned…and [I] totally even forgot by the end of it that they
mentioned global warming because it was just in passing.
Environmental messages that may be present in disaster cli-fi films such as this are thus diluted
by the ambiguous relationship between disasters and climate change and the lack of a concrete
mechanism linking the two.
Speed and severity of disasters
Participants expressed skepticism as to whether the natural disasters depicted could unfold with
the speed and severity depicted in the films. Several participants noted the extreme nature of the
disasters made the film seem less realistic and credible.
PARTICIPANT 21: [I]t loses all credibility with just the magnitude of how frequent these
tornadoes are…The chance of this happening is extremely low, so it loses
all its credibility when you’re pushing it to that extent.
One participant suggested the extreme and unusual disasters may lead to audiences’ dismissing
not only the film itself, but climate science as sensationalized.
PARTICIPANT 1: Well, global warming causes more extreme weather and…hurricanes or
tornadoes and things like that. I don’t think you got anything this extreme
where a tornado shoots lightning. So, people who watch would be, “Oh,
that’s not real global warming, that’s exaggerated.”
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
142
Others felt that the disasters’ rapid speed damaged the films’ realism.
PARTICIPANT 5: I feel like it happened too fast.
PARTICIPANT 12: [W]ith the science…let’s not forget that this all happened in the span of a
day.
PARTICIPANT 17: It was really drastic. “Oh, nice day,” and then the tornadoes show up and
then the storm would go about it and then it turned into completely gray
sky. It really cannot happen that fast, I'm pretty sure that it doesn't happen
in 3 minutes.
The disasters’ rapid speed added to the lack of scientific context to make the film less impactful
for one participant:
PARTICIPANT 18: I just didn't like the fact that they were…I think they [the main characters]
were at a party or something like that, and these things [the tornadoes] just
come up out of nowhere. So, can you at least tell us what led to it over the
years? To me they should have done something to lead up to that…It
scares me for a second and then it's just, “Can this possibly happen?”
Exaggerating the speed and severity of natural disasters associated with climate change appears
to damage external realism for audiences, potentially making the films less likely to impact their
attitudes and beliefs.
Representation of science in disaster cli-fi films
The depiction of science was a significant source of skepticism and uncertainty for participants.
Many participants found the science and underlying concepts unrealistic. When asked how
accurate the film’s science was, some participants outright rejected it:
PARTICIPANT 21: Literally zero. There’s nothing behind it.
PARTICIPANT 19: I think the majority of the stuff we saw in the film was really unrealistic.
Similar to Lowe et al.’s results (Lowe et al., 2006), some participants expressed uncertainty
as to the boundary between factual science and film science. Despite criticizing the films for lack
of scientific realism, comments made by several participants reveal a degree of uncertainty
regarding science fact vs. fiction. Many participants admitted they were unsure whether the
phenomena depicted—such as St. Elmo’s fire and cloud seeding—were possible. They expressed
surprise when other participants pointed out that these were not entirely fictionalized concepts,
expressing tentative acceptance of the fictionalized science.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
143
PARTICIPANT 11: I thought it was pretty cool, the idea that, with the mini tornadoes, it’s kind
of cool. I didn’t know that they get cold on the inside, though.
PARTICIPANT 14: I don’t know too much about extreme weather, so, I mean to me just the
idea that something like that could potentially happen is really crazy.
PARTICIPANT 1: [I]t’s something that could happen, hypothetically.
PARTICIPANT 3: I’m not an expert but I think anything’s possible.
Other participants were more ambivalent:
PARTICIPANT 2: Could be possible. One of the things that stood out to me was that…they
never gave much of an explanation as to what was going on. The news
kept saying that it was a side effect of global warming.
PARTICIPANT 8: I was thinking about the high-low atmosphere splitting and things like
that. I don’t know if that’s a thing, but that just sounds more
believable.…So there were certain aspects of it, you know, that I feel were
possible.
PARTICIPANT 23: I feel like lightning can definitely kill people and if it’s powerful enough,
can cause a fire or whatever, but I don’t know. Maybe small parts of it
were realistic, but most of it wasn’t.
PARTICIPANT 25: It’s just the concept in general that was interesting, that one massive storm
could overtake…I wasn’t sure. It didn’t seem really real. I don’t know in
terms of the scientific part of it, to determine if it’s actually feasible.
Several participants explained they were unqualified to judge the films’ accuracy without
further research.
PARTICIPANT 11: I don’t have anything to compare it to yet [in terms of accuracy]. I haven’t
researched it.
PARTICIPANT 18: I don't know if something like this could be a possibility. I would have to
do research on it to speak on it, but it's definitely scary if it can happen. So
[it] definitely scares people.
Many participants said the film disasters were too outlandish and vague to look up on their
own, but others expressed curiosity. In line with Hart and Leiserowitz’s findings on informationseeking
among The Day After Tomorrow audiences (Hart & Leiserowitz, 2009), participants
expressed interest in learning more about the disasters in the film. I asked participants what, if
anything, they would look up after the discussion.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
144
PARTICIPANT 12: How much of that [the tornadoes in the film] is actually true.
PARTICIPANT 15: Are there any tornados [such as those shown] that actually happened?
Participants also critiqued the films’ representation of science, particularly the lone scientist
trope. The mysterious disasters in all three films are eventually understood and/or destroyed by a
single scientist character who races the clock with his/her research. Participants correctly argue
that science is frequently a collaborative enterprise, making it unlikely that a lone scientist could
successfully solve such a complex problem on his/her own.
PARTICIPANT 4: I think there would be a lot more people involved, a lot more.
PARTICIPANT 16: She was the only one who came up with something. I guess she was the
head and she was the smartest one…But I mean, I was wondering, “What
were those people [the rest of the scientific staff] doing?”
Likewise, participants criticized the speed with which the lead scientist identified the
problem and developed a solution. These critiques were often related to participants’ perceptions
that the disaster evolved too quickly:
PARTICIPANT 11: Science does not take a day.
PARTICIPANT 12: [T]here would be a lot more testing and stuff going on, especially if it is
some kind of disaster…But they were so sure – at least she was – so sure
that this is exactly the perfect solution, and you don’t get that sort of
certainty in that short of time.
PARTICIPANT 11: And remember two hours before [revealing the solution to the tornado
outbreak] she [the lead scientist] was like, “This will take years.” Two
seconds later – done. No.
Efficacy and disaster cli-fi films
One important criticism participants had of the film was that it did not contain any suggestions as
to how to combat anthropogenic climate change in the real world. Participants felt the
environmental message of the film was weakened by telling them a story about the dangers of
climate change but failing to provide any suggestions for concrete actions audience members
could take in their own lives to mitigate the threat.
PARTICIPANT 5: I think this film in particular didn’t really address that [taking action to
minimize the threat] because they didn’t give an explanation for why it
was out there. They just jumped right in there. There wasn’t, “Oh there’s
so much greenhouse gas,” or “Oh you’re using too much fossil fuels.”
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
145
PARTICIPANT 14: [T]hey didn’t really go deeper in terms of this is why it’s global warming
specifically…And [there was] no message about, “We need to fix what
we’re doing or something.” They’re just, “Oh let’s stop this, what’s going
on now.”
PARTICIPANT 8: I feel like it doesn’t say any of the techniques [to stop climate change] – if
someone was to, you know, think it was their fault.
This criticism is in line with other studies in the social sciences that argue the lack of concrete
suggestions for how to combat climate change weakens environmental messages (Hart &
Feldman, 2014; Li, 2014).
Impacts on environmental attitudes
The consensus was that these films are not particularly effective at changing environmental
attitudes, particularly in light of the criticisms regarding lack of efficacy. Although some
participants suggested that less educated, less informed members of the public would be more
likely to take the films at face value, participants generally felt that the films were too inaccurate
to impact attitudes. Some participants even went as far as to say films such as the ones used in
the study made people less likely to accept anthropogenic climate change as a real threat for
which action is required.
PARTICIPANT 2: I think if people would see this movie they would kind of think that global
warming is not real because of the movie.
PARTICIPANT 13: I mean from this [film], they probably think of it as a joke.
That being said, participants did not outright reject the idea that fictional films could have
indirect impacts on attitudes. Even though viewers might dismiss the films as unrealistic, a
lingering uncertainty could remain, as some participants noted:
PARTICIPANT 4: I think that people might not believe it, but then still have the doubt that it
could happen.
PARTICIPANT 2: I think in the back of their mind.
Because participants had difficulty distinguishing where fact and fiction diverged, the
possibility of extreme disasters seemed to remain for some participants.
PARTICIPANT 18: [O]f course you're going to have people that say, “Oh Syfy, just another
one of those movies.” Then you have some people that at first hand, will
get kind of scared, because they're like, “Well, I don't know much about
science,” like myself, and they'll think, “Well, maybe this possibly can
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
146
happen, even though it's a movie.” But for a while people might think,
“Maybe this can happen.”
Discussion
Several conclusions can be drawn from these results. Participants generally noticed recurring
mentions of climate change but were divided on whether these constituted a major theme. Much
of this hesitancy came from the lack of a detailed, specific discussion of climate change within
the films. Interestingly, the few participants with a skeptical attitude toward anthropogenic
climate change tended to be most adamant that climate change was a theme. It may be that such
participants were not sufficiently transported into the world of the narrative to preclude back
arguing against the films’ premises. It may also be that participants who have strong feelings of
climate skepticism may have picked up on climate change references early on because they
perceived such references as an effort to challenge their deeply-held beliefs.
The predominant emotional responses to the films were incredulity and confusion.
Underneath this umbrella of skepticism were three specific areas of disbelief: the causal
relationship between climate change and disasters (violation of internal realism), the swiftness
and severity of the portrayed disasters (violation of external realism), and the representation of
science (violation of external realism).
Despite their generally skeptical attitude toward the depiction of science and climate change
in the films, participants remained uncertain about the barrier between science fact and science
fiction. Even though participants openly expressed disbelief that the events in the film could
happen, many seemed unsure as to the degree to which the films exaggerated and invented
phenomena. This uncertainty suggests participants struggled to pinpoint the specific point at
which the films departed from established science, similar to the results found by Lowe et al.
(2006).
It appears the violations of internal and external realism documented impacted audience
involvement in the films, as predicted by the literature. The combination of extreme,
scientifically-questionable events and weak causation in disaster cli-fi films caused audiences to
lose track of the climate change theme. In addition, participants questioned the sped-up timelines
and exaggerated intensity of the disasters in the films. Unrealistic timelines and impossible
impacts from climate change are common criticisms of disaster cli-fi films levied by both
filmgoers and scientists alike (Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al., 2006). Participants felt the use of a
lone scientist trope, where a single individual diagnoses and solves the disaster on his/her own,
was inaccurate. They suggested these features made the film less realistic and weakened their
engagement with the storyline. This is in line with other research which suggests that films must
maintain a certain level of believability to ensure that audiences remain engaged (Lowe et al.,
2006).
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
147
Overall, participants doubted the films’ ability to affect environmental attitudes and beliefs.
They argued the films were too inaccurate and lacking in credibility to influence how the public
thinks about climate change. These findings contradict other research on environmental attitudes
and fictional films. Although the participants dismissed the influence of these particular films on
environmental attitudes, studies have shown that The Day After Tomorrow (2004) did affect
audiences’ attitudes toward climate change (Howell, 2011; Leiserowitz, 2004; Lowe et al.,
2006).
There are a few possible explanations for the disconnect between what participants say
about the impacts of film on environmental attitudes and what researchers have measured. The
third-person effect is a well-documented phenomenon within communications studies (Davison,
1983) which “predicts that individuals will perceive media messages to have greater effects on
other people than on themselves” (Salwen & Dupagne, 1999, p. 523). The third-person effect is
thought to arise because people generally believe they are smarter than others; thus, they are less
likely to be persuaded by the media than others who are less intelligent (Paul, Salwen, &
Dupagne, 2000). Given the participants’ observation that the natural disasters depicted in disaster
cli-fi films are highly unscientific and exaggerated, it could be that participants assumed that no
one was gullible enough to be influenced by them. Theoretically, this would suggest a sort of
floor to the third-person effect whereby some forms of media are seen as so obviously absurd
that no one could be influenced by them—even the assumed less-intelligent third-person.
I suggest that what is more likely is that participants were more influenced by the films than
they openly acknowledged. Indeed, given the uncertainty expressed by many participants as to
where scientific fact stopped and science fiction began, the films may have impacted participant
environmental attitudes without their explicit awareness. This impact would support the findings
of Lowe et al. (2006) and Leiserowitz (2004), as well as the concerns that audiences will mistake
fictional science for science facts (Kirby, 2003b, 2003a). Studies that suggest people struggle to
separate fact from fiction when watching science fiction films (Barriga et al., 2010) and
historical fiction films (Butler et al., 2009; Marsh et al., 2012, 2003) add to this fear.
Thus, it is worth considering the possibility that, although fictional films are unlikely to
sway climate deniers and unlikely to shake supporters of the consensus, subtler environmental
beliefs and scientific understandings of issues like climate change could be affected. These
misunderstandings have implications for climate change communicators and advocates, who
may find themselves spending more time debunking misperceptions. Although more research is
needed, it is possible that the inaccurate science in these films can contribute to a general lack of
understanding of what causes climate change, how humans are contributing, and why climate
change is a threat.
Equally concerning is the possibility that film audiences, having been exposed to inaccurate
ideas about climate change and storms in disaster cli-fi, may be more susceptible to
misinformation in the event of actual storms. During the preparations for Hurricane Irma in
September 2017, for example, an image of a fake news ticker with the caption “Irma Now
Contains Sharks” went viral on social media (Porter, 2017). Irma was the subject of a variety of
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
148
hoaxes, including the rumor that it would make landfall in Florida as a Category 6 storm
(hurricanes are measured on the Saffir-Simpson scale, which goes up to 5) (Ohlheiser, 2017). It
may be that people who saw these films misremembered the inaccurate, fictional storm
information later, making them more receptive to misleading claims during an actual extreme
weather event. More research is needed to determine to what extent, if any, inaccurate
information in cli-fi influences perceptions of real disasters. However, as was evident during
Irma, debunking such misinformation falls on the shoulders of meteorologists and other public
safety officials whose time would be better spent warning of actual dangers.
Limitations and directions for future research
Using undergraduate students for focus groups poses certain limitations, as these students are not
representative of movie audiences in the United States or elsewhere. It is also possible younger
media consumers have different levels of scientific education and understanding than older
media consumers, impacting what they see as realistic or unrealistic. Further studies should
explore how a wider audience interacts with disaster cli-fi films to see whether these results hold
true for other demographics.
Audiences exposed to historical fiction films often incorrectly interpret historical
fabrications as fact (Butler et al., 2009; Marsh et al., 2012, 2003). Whether such
misinterpretations occur with disaster cli-fi films as well is an area of study that would be quite
fruitful and important. Specifically, if audiences are absorbing misleading or false information
about climate change from popular culture, science communicators may be forced to devote
increasing amounts of time and energy to debunking false notions propagated in fiction.
Other studies exploring the influence of film on environmental attitudes have suggested
films can affect attitudes and beliefs about climate change (Howell, 2011; Leiserowitz, 2004;
Lowe et al., 2006). These studies also suggest fictional representations of climate change in film
can influence audiences’ intended actions on climate change. Because of the limited scope of
this study, it is not possible to say whether the low-budget disaster cli-fi films such as the ones
examined here have similar effects on audience actions. Given the proliferation of these films in
the media marketplace, it would be useful for science communicators to understand how these
films impact their audiences’ willingness to act on climate change.
Although it has been over a decade since the theatrical release of The Day After Tomorrow
(2004), new examples of climate change in popular culture are multiplying rapidly. The
Sharknado series—which specifically invokes climate change—has become a cult classic, with
five films total in the series as well as books, documentaries, and a plethora of merchandise. Clifi
is rapidly becoming an important genre in film and other media. Social scientists who wish to
study public attitudes on climate change will increasingly need to engage with pop culture. My
findings suggest the relationship between pop culture and attitudes is complex but specific
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
149
components of film and fiction may be more important than others in determining audience
reactions.
References
Barriga, C. A., Shapiro, M. A., & Fernandez, M. L. (2010). Science information in fictional
movies: Effects of context and gender. Science Communication, 32(1), 3–24.
doi:10.1177/1075547009340338
Beattie, G., Sale, L., & McGuire, L. (2011). An inconvenient truth? Can a film really affect
psychological mood and our explicit attitudes towards climate change? Semiotica, 187(1–4),
105–125. doi:10.1515/semi.2011.066
Bloom, D. (2017). The Cli-Fi Report. Retrieved from http://cli-fi.net/
Boykoff, J. M., & Boykoff, M. T. (2007). Climate change and journalistic norms: A case-study
of US mass-media coverage. Geoforum, 38(6), 1190–1204.
doi:10.1016/j.geoforum.2007.01.008
Boykoff, M. T., & Rajan, S. R. (2007). Signals and noise. Mass-media coverage of climate
change in the USA and the UK. EMBO Reports, 8(3), 207–211.
doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400924
Braddock, K., & Dillard, J. P. (2016). Meta-analytic evidence for the persuasive effect of
narratives on beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Communication Monographs,
83(4), 446–467. doi:10.1080/03637751.2015.1128555
Braddock, K., & Horgan, J. (2016). Towards a guide for constructing and disseminating
counternarratives to reduce support for terrorism. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 39(5),
381–404. doi:10.1080/1057610X.2015.1116277
Brulle, R. J., Carmichael, J., & Jenkins, J. C. (2012). Shifting public opinion on climate change:
An empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the U.S.,
2002-2010. Climatic Change, 114(1), 169. doi:10.1007/s10584-012-0403-y
Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories:
A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18(2), 255–
280. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00322.x
Busselle, R. W., & Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychology,
12(4), 321–347. doi:10.1080/15213260903287259
Butler, A. C., Zaromb, F. M., Lyle, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Using popular films to
enhance classroom learning: The good, the bad, and the interesting. Psychological Science,
20(9), 1161–1168. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02410.x
Caputo, N. M., & Rouner, D. (2011). Narrative processing of entertainment media and mental
illness stigma. Health Communication, 26(7), 595–604. doi:10.1080/10410236.2011.560787
Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative
analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
150
Charmaz, K. (2008). Grounded theory as an emergent method. In S. N. Hesse-Biber & P. Leavy
(Eds.), Handbook of emergent methods. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Corbett, J., & Durfee, J. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science: Media representations of
global warming. Science Communication, 26(2), 129–151. doi:10.1177/1075547004270234
Dahlstrom, M. F. (2010). The role of causality in information acceptance in narratives: An
example from science communication. Communication Research, 37(6), 857–875.
doi:10.1177/0093650210362683
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. The Public Opinion
Quarterly, 47(1), 1–15. doi:10.1086/268763
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public
narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701–721.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.701
Hall, A. (2003). Reading realism: Audiences’ evaluations of the reality of media texts. Journal of
Communication, 53(4), 624–641. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb02914.x
Hart, P. S., & Leiserowitz, A. (2009). Finding the teachable moment: An analysis of
information-seeking behavior on global warming related websites during the release of The
Day After Tomorrow. Environmental Communication - A Journal of Nature and Culture,
3(3), 355–366. doi:10.1080/17524030903265823
Howell, R. A. (2011). Lights, camera...action? Altered attitudes and behaviour in response to the
climate change film The Age of Stupid. Global Environmental Change, 21(1), 177–187.
doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.09.004
Kahan, D. (2012). Why we are poles apart on climate change: The problem isn’t the public’s
reasoning capacity; it’s the polluted science-communication environment that drives people
apart. Nature, 488(7411), 255. doi:10.1038/488255a
Kahan, D., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus.
Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174. doi:10.1080/13669877.2010.511246
Kaplan, E. A. (2015). Climate trauma: Foreseeing the future in dystopian film and fiction. New
Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Kirby, D. A. (2003a). Science consultants, fictional films, and scientific practice. Social Studies
of Science, 33(2), 231–268. doi:10.1177/03063127030332015
Kirby, D. A. (2003b). Scientists on the set: Science consultants and the communication of
science in visual fiction. Public Understanding of Science, 12(3), 261–278.
doi:10.1177/0963662503123005
Konijn, E. A., van der Molen, J. H. W., & van Nes, S. (2009). Emotions bias perceptions of
realism in audiovisual media: Why we may take fiction for real. Discourse Processes, 46(4),
309–340. doi:10.1080/01638530902728546
Krueger, R. A., & Casey, M. A. (2009). Focus groups: A practical guide for applied research.
Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications.
Leiserowitz, A. (2004). Before and after the day after tomorrow: A US study of climate change
risk perception. Environment, 46(9), 22–37.
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
151
Lowe, T., Brown, K., Dessai, S., de Franca Doria, M., Haynes, K., & Vincent, K. (2006). Does
tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public perceptions of climate change. Public
Understanding of Science, 15(4), 435–457. doi:10.1177/0963662506063796
Marsh, E. J., Butler, A. C., & Umanath, S. (2012). Using fictional sources in the classroom:
Applications from cognitive psychology. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 449–469.
doi:10.1007/s10648-012-9204-0
Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger III, H. L. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of
Memory and Language, 49(4), 519–536. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00092-5
Merchant, B. (2013). Behold the Rise of Dystopian “Cli-Fi.” Retrieved fromhttps://motherboard.vice.com/en_us/article/ypp7nj/behold-the-rise-of-cli-fi
Morgan, D. L. (1993). Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art. Newbury Park,
CA: Sage Publications.
Morgan, D. L. (1996). Focus groups. Annual Review of Sociology, 22(1), 129–152.
doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.22.1.129
Moyer-Guse, E. (2008). Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: Explaining the persuasive
effects of entertainment-education messages. Communication Theory, 18(3), 407–425.
doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00328.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2011). An experimental test of the effects of fictional framing on
attitudes. Social Science Quarterly, 92(1), 79–99. doi:10.1111/j.1540-6237.2011.00758.x
Mulligan, K., & Habel, P. (2013). The implications of fictional media for political beliefs.
American Politics Research, 41(1), 122–146. doi:10.1177/1532673X12453758
Murphy, P. (2014). Directing the weather, producing the climate. In Alex, R.S., Deborah, S.S., &
Sachindev, P.S. (Eds.), Culture and media: Ecocritical explorations. Newcastle upon Tyne,
UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Murray, R. L., & Heumann, J. K. (2009). Ecology and popular film: Cinema on the edge.
Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Ohlheiser, A. (2017, September 11). Analysis | A running list of viral hoaxes about Irma —
including one shared by the White House. The Washington Post Retrieved fromhttps://www.washingtonpost.com
Parker, T. (2006). ManBearPig. Comedy Central. Retrieved from http://southpark.cc.com/fullepisodes/
s10e06-manbearpig
Paul, B., Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (2000). The third-person effect: A meta-analysis of the
perceptual hypothesis. Mass Communication and Society, 3(1), 57–85.
doi:10.1207/S15327825MCS0301_04
Porter, T. (2017, September 9). Here’s some of the fake news and false rumors about Hurricane
Irma. Retrieved from http://www.newsweek.com/hurricane-irma-fake-news-false-rumorsspread-
storm-662419
Rooney, B., Benson, C., & Hennessy, E. (2012). The apparent reality of movies and emotional
arousal: A study using physiological and self-report measures. Poetics, 40, 405–422.
doi:10.1016/j.poetic.2012.07.004
Griffin, Audience Reactions to Climate Change, JPIC, Vol. 1 Issue 2 (2017)
152
Salwen, M. B., & Dupagne, M. (1999). The third-person effect. Communication Research, 26(5),
523. doi: 10.1177/009365099026005001
Shen, F., Ahern, L., & Baker, M. (2014). Stories that count: Influence of news narratives on
issue attitudes. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 91(1), 98–117.
doi:10.1177/1077699013514414
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). The rise of global warming skepticism: Exploring affective
image associations in the United States over time. Risk Analysis, 32(6), 1021–1032.
doi:10.1605/01.301-0019863810.2012
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2014). The role of emotion in global warming policy support and
opposition. Risk Analysis, 34(5), 937–948. doi:10.1111/risa.12140
Svoboda, M. (2014). The Long Melt: The Lingering Influence of The Day After Tomorrow.
Retrieved from http://www.yaleclimateconnections.org/2014/11/the-long-melt-thelingering-
influence-of-the-day-after-tomorrow/
Svoboda, M. (2016). Cli-fi on the screen(s): Patterns in the representations of climate change in
fictional films. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 7(1), 43–64.
doi:10.1002/wcc.381
Tan, E. S. H. (2008). Entertainment is emotion: The functional architecture of the entertainment
experience. Media Psychology, 11(1), 28–51. doi:10.1080/15213260701853161
No comments:
Post a Comment